نوع مقاله : مقاله پژوهشی
نویسندگان
1 دانشیار زبان شناسی کاربردی، دانشگاه خوارزمی
2 استاد زبانشناسی کاربردی، دانشگاه خوارزمی
3 دانشجوی دکتری آموزش زبان انگلیسی، دانشگاه خوارزمی
چکیده
تحقیقات در مورد نگارش آکادمیک نشان داده است که نویسندگان تمایل زیادی دارند تا به صورت تعاملی یافتههای علمی خود را با خوانندگان خود درمیان بگذارند. در انجام این کار، نویسندگان بایستی موضع خود را هنگام نوشتن مقالات علمی مشخص کنند. این برهمکنش که توسط هایلند پیشنهاد شده است دارای دو بعد موضع نویسنده و تعامل است. مطالعه حاضر با ادغام چارچوب هایلند و هایلند و سه برای بررسی تعامل نویسنده و خواننده، نشانگرهای واژگانی و دستوری موضع نویسنده را در بخش های اصلی مقالات پژوهشی انگلیسی در علوم مردم شناسی، آموزش، باغبانی و جانورشناسی در نظر دارد. پیکره تحقیق شامل 240 مقاله پژوهشی انگلیسی است که طی دو دوره منتشر شدهاند، یعنی 1990 و 2010. 60 مقاله از هر رشته، 30 مقاله از سال 1990 و 30 مقاله از 2010 با مجموع 1270021 کلمه .یافته های این پژوهش را تشکیل میدهد. صرف نظر از ماهیت رشته، موضعگیری نویسنده ویژگی مشترکی از نگارش آکادمیک در رشته های نمونهگیری شده را تشکیل داد. همچنین، عبارتهای احتیاطآمیز از نظر بسامدی رتبه اول را به خود اختصاص دادند .علاوه بر این، الگویی کاهشی در استفاده از نشانگرهای موضعی وجود داشت که همگرایی میان محققان این حوزهها را نشان میدهد. در پایان، چند نکته با کاربرد محتمل در نگارش آکادمیک و طراحی مطالب مربوط به انگلیسی برای اهداف آکادمیک پیشنهاد شده است.
کلیدواژهها
Discourse analysis has witnessed a gradual shift from a limited and limiting view on texts as ideational units of thought to a more interpersonal function (Hyland, 2004) giving birth to various models of language analysis including metadiscourse. As the foundation stone of the present research, metadiscourse has witnessed the waxes and wanes like other areas of science. As claimed by Hyland (2005a), the term metadiscourse was coined by Zellig Harris in 1959 to represent a writer’s or speaker’s attempt to guide a receiver’s perception of a text. The concept was later developed by Williams (1981) as writing about writing and used to refer to whatever other than the subject matter being addressed. It has also been defined as discourse which goes beyond and above the actual content of the basic propositional information being presented, indicating to readers how they may “organize, classify, interpret, evaluate and react to” (Vande Kopple, 1985, p. 83) information presented in the text. Crismore (1989) has regarded metadiscourse as a “social, rhetorical instrument which can be used pragmatically to get things done” (p. 4).
Later in its development, Hyland (2005a) defines metadiscourse as “the self-reflective expressions used to negotiate interactional meanings in a text, assisting the writer (or speaker) to express a viewpoint and engage with readers as members of a particular community” (p. 37). In other words, metadiscourse deals with a range of rhetorical resources employed (and sometimes exploited!) to link the tripartite of the writer, the evolving text and the intended reader (Hyland, 2009). Accordingly, due to this significant connecting role, researchers working on writing, especially academic and scholarly writing, have paid growing attention to such resources in the past decade (e.g., Abdi, Rizi, & Tavakoli, 2010; Hyland, 2005a; Hyland and Tse, 2004; Peterlin, 2005).
This growing focus of interest has led to an increasing recognition that, as opposed to common belief, academic writing cannot be considered as exclusively objective and factual (Jalilifar, 2014). There are features within the text that encode the writer’s point of view and take the role of mediators between the information presented in the text and the writer’s factual information (Hyland, 2010). In other words, academic writers “actively draw from a range of rhetorical strategies rooted in their own disciplines and socio-cultural milieus to organize arguments, provide evidence, and evaluate claims to convince their readers” (Hu & Cao, 2011, p. 2795). As Hyland (2005b) himself asserts, much of his and his colleagues’ work (e.g., Hyland, 2005a; Hyland & Tse, 2004, Hyland & Tse, 2005a, 2005b) over the past decade or so has been devoted to shed more light on the shift academic writing has experienced with regard to its potential objectives and missions in the eyes of both its writers and target readers. Over the past decades “academic writing has gradually lost its traditional tag as an objective, faceless and impersonal form of discourse and come to be seen as a persuasive endeavour involving interaction between writers and readers” (Hyland, 2005a, p. 173). As such, scholarly writing has been viewed “as evaluative and interpersonal rather than simply informational and objective” (Hyland & Tse, 2005a, p. 39).
LITERATURE REVIEW
When it comes to a discussion of broad approaches to writing, one can easily discern a transition from text, to writer, and ultimately to the reader (Hyland, 2009). Put simply, text-oriented writing takes writing as product; writer-oriented writing is concerned with writing as process; and reader-oriented writing takes the social dimension into account. It can be claimed that metadiscourse has been founded upon the reader-oriented stage of writing development. Therefore, in terms of social and interpersonal engagements, metadiscourse resources play a significant role. This is obviously noticeable in the distinction made between interactive and interactional metadiscourse (Hyland & Tse, 2004). In a nutshell, the former organizes the texts and directs the reader through them, while the latter engages and orients the reader through the propositional content postulated by the writer (Hyland, 2005a).
Hyland (2005b) has moved even a step further and proposed a model to account for the interaction in academic writing which is basically positioning or alignment from two perspectives: one is picking a point of view on the issues discussed and the other is having an eye on the involvement of the members of the discourse community in the issues raised; the former dubbed as stance while the latter as engagement. Figure 1 shows his model schematically.
Figure 1. Key components of academic interaction
Hyland (2004, 2005b) uses the term stance or author stance in a broader way. According to Hyland (2005b), stance:
can be seen as an attitudinal dimension and includes features which refer to the ways writers present themselves and convey their judgments, opinions, and commitments. It is the ways that writers intrude to stamp their personal authority onto their arguments or step back and disguise their involvement (p. 176).
As it can be observed in Figure 1, in Hyland’s (2005b) conceptualization, writers try to put their voice into their writing by applying the subcomponents of stance. Categorically speaking, its subcomponents can be defined and exemplified as follows (summarized from Hyland, 2007, pp. 94-95):
- Hedges (devices which withhold complete commitment to a proposition), for example, possible, may, could, tendency.
- Boosters (devices which allow writers to express their certainty in what they say and to mark involvement with the topic and solidarity with their audience), for example, should, definitely, of course.
- Attitude markers (devices which indicate the writer’s affective, rather than epistemic, attitude to propositions, conveying surprise, agreement, importance, frustration, and so on, rather than commitment), for example, believe, remarkable, extraordinary, interesting.
- Self-mentions (the use of first person pronouns and possessive adjectives to present information), for example, I, we, our.
Various attempts have been made, first, to explore stance features used and strategies employed by writers to build solidarity with their readers and, second, to devise a framework to analyze these features and strategies. To do so, Hyland and Tse (2004), for example, analyzed 240 L2 postgraduate dissertations totaling 4 million words including 20 master’s and 20 doctoral dissertations from six academic disciplines: electronic engineering, computer science, business studies, biology, applied linguistics, and public administration. In addition to some detailed findings regarding the meaningful and significantly different metadiscourse use by certain disciplines, the general finding was that “metadiscourse offers a way of understanding the interpersonal resources writers use to present propositional material and therefore a means of uncovering something of the rhetorical and social distinctiveness of disciplinary communities” (Hyland & Tse, 2004, p. 156). Gillaerts and Van de Velde (2010) conducted a qualitative study to investigate how writers deploy stance markers, namely hedges, boosters and attitude markers. The study was diachronic in nature because it examined three decades of abstract writing in the field of applied linguistics. In fact, they aimed to investigate to what extent research article abstracts differ with regard to the use of the above mentioned stance markers and whether any changes have occurred in the past 3 decades. The corpus included 72 research article abstracts from articles in Journal of Pragmatics taken from different volumes starting in 2007 and going back to 1982. Based on the results, it was claimed that research article and research article abstracts show differential use of subcategories of stance markers in focus. That is, “whereas research articles exhibit a rather high number of hedges in comparison to boosters and attitude markers, abstracts show more affinity with boosting, rather than with hedging” (Gillaerts & Van de Velde, 2010, p. 135).
Regarding the second aim of the study i.e., diachronic changes in the use of stance markers in research article abstracts, their study revealed that “the use of interactional metadiscourse has dropped in three consecutive decades, mainly as a result of a reduction in the number of boosters and attitude markers” (p. 136). It was argued that the possible reason for such decline in the use of stance markers might be due to a converging move of applied linguistics towards the hard sciences in which, as Hyland (2005b) asserts, the use of interactional metadiscourse is markedly lower.
Hu and Cao (2011) examined if there were cross-cultural and cross-linguistic differences in the use of hedges and boosters in academic article abstracts. They also aimed to report any possible differences between authors of empirical and non-empirical academic articles. More precisely, they aimed to find any differences and/or similarities in the use of hedges and boosters between abstracts published in major Chinese- and English-medium academic journals in the discipline of applied linguistics. Exploring any differences and/or similarities in the use of hedges and boosters between abstracts accompanying empirical and non-empirical articles was the second aim of the study. To do so, a corpus of 649 abstracts was collected from 8 English- and Chinese-medium journals of applied linguistics. Considering the fact that Chinese journals carried abstracts written in both Chinese and English for each article, three subcorpora were detected for the study: (a) the CA-CJ subcorpus (i.e., Chinese abstracts published in the Chinese journals), (b) the EA-CJ subcorpus (i.e., English abstracts published in the Chinese journals), and (c) the EA-EJ subcorpus (i.e., English abstracts published in the English journals). Generally speaking, cross-cultural/linguistic contrast was reported across the subcorpora. Markedly lesser hedges and somewhat greater boosters were used in CA-CJ subcorpus compared with EA-EJ subcorpus. Consequently, it was claimed that “the abstracts in the CA-CJ subcorpus tended to express a notably higher degree of certainty, confidence and/or commitment than the abstracts in the EA-EJ subcorpus” (Hu & Cao, 2011, p. 2804). It was also reported that “the English abstracts in the Chinese-medium journals used significantly fewer boosters than their matching Chinese abstracts, but did not differ statistically from the abstracts in the English-medium journals” (pp. 2805-2806). Regarding the empirical and non-empirical distinction, the study revealed that the abstracts of the empirical RAs employed significantly more boosters than those of the non-empirical academic articles.
In an attempt to investigate cultural variations in the use of metadiscourse in academic writing including stance markers, Onder Ozdemira and Longo (2014) compared the amounts and types of metadiscourse markers employed by Turkish and US postgraduate students’ abstracts in MA thesis written in English. Drawing on Hyland’s (2005a) metadiscourse taxonomy, the researchers took both interactive and interactional resources into account. Employing quantitative and qualitative analyses, they reported some cultural similarities and differences in the MA abstracts of Turkish and American students. Generally speaking, the “frequency of interactive and particularly interactional metadiscourse was higher in the American students’ abstracts than Turkish students, 925.8 and 855.8, respectively” (Onder Ozdemira & Longo, 2014, p. 61). Considering interactive resources, they reported that the frequency of evidential, code glosses and endophorics was very low in Turkish postgraduate students’ abstracts compared with American postgraduate students’ abstracts. Among interactive metadiscourse markers, transitions and frame markers were the most frequently used ones by both Turkish and American students. Regarding interactional metadiscourse, no significant difference was reported in the frequency of hedges used by Turkish and American students. American students significantly employed more boosters than Turkish students i.e., American students used boosters 26 times more than Turkish students. Likewise, the frequency of attitude markers was higher for American students. Self-mentions were not used by Turkish students; whereas, very few were used by American students. All in all, considering the frequencies in the interactive and interactional metadiscourse, the study of Onder Ozdemira and Longo (2014) revealed that “US students used interactional metadiscourse twice more (112.1 vs. 271), while the use of interactive metadiscourse was similar (743.7 vs. 654.8)” (p. 62). This difference and similarity can be attributed to non-native students’ (Turkish students) inability in controling the degree of personality in argumentation including statement of affinity and attitude.
Based on an analysis of 240 published research papers, Hyland (2014) has argued that stance features like hedges, self-mentions, and boosters “are not simply dry textualisations but elements of persuasive craftsmanship which help construct a disciplinary view of the world while simultaneously negotiating a credible persona for writers” (p. 1). Put differently, academic writers make use of stance features to wave their discourse community flag primarily to make their own voice audible and secondarily make their judgments and claims more persuasive and convincing.
In addition to the above mentioned stance markers which are lexical in nature, writers may also deploy grammatical stance markers. One realization of such grammatical stancetaking is evaluative that construction by which the writers can express their stance explicitly or implicitly. This construction is described as evaluative that referring to “a structure which allows a writer to thematize attitudinal meanings and present an explicit statement of evaluation by presenting a complement clause within a super-ordinate clause” (Hyland & Tse, 2005a, p. 39). Writers can use different types of that construction with verbs (. . . it is hypothesized that….), adjectives (I am certain that….), and nouns (One important finding of this study is that….) as controling words (Hyland & Tse, 2005a). To the best knowledge of the researchers, the single relevant study has been conducted by Hyland and Tse (2005b). They have investigated evaluative that structure in 240 research article abstracts from six disciplines. The disciplines under focus were applied linguistics (AL), biology (Bio), business studies (BS), computer science (CS), electrical engineering (EE), and public administration (PA). With regard to frequency, all in all, evaluative that occurred almost seven times per thousand words. Taking disciplinary variations into account, the biology sample contained the highest examples of evaluative that construction (as raw frequency) and computer science abstracts had the highest number when texts were normalized to 1,000 words. Social sciences, applied linguistics and public administration, were either around or just below the overall average per paper. Electronic engineers made the least use of that constructions with only ten occurrences overall.
In view of the above, among academia more focus is placed on stance side of the coin as there is a general consensus that any writing particularly academic writing is about argumentation and persuasion (e.g., Jalilifar, 2014). Therefore, the first side of the dichotomy looms large for research purposes which is reflected in the logic behind the present study focusing on the stance side of the coin. Consequently, the present study enjoys some unique features.
PURPOSE OF THE STUDY
The present study aimed at exploring lexical and grammatical stance markers in the main subsections of RAs employing a comparative lens to take account of disciplinarity and date of publication. As RAs are the primary means of knowledge sharing and dissemination and subject to change over time like other instances of genres (Bazerman, 1988), it was hypothesized that time passage could be a defining factor when it comes to RA writing.
Despite the studies looking differently at the concept of stancetaking and stance markers as reviewed above, further research is required to carefully appraise the use of stance markers in subsections of RAs over time and across disciplines. Therefore, unlike previous studies investigating individual sections of RAs such as Abstract, Introduction, Discussion, and Conclusion (e.g., Abdi, 2002; Abdollahzadeh, 2011; Del Saz Rubio, 2011; Khedri, Heng, & Ebrahimi, 2013), or on the “post-method” sections (Cao & Hu, 2014), the current study, integrating Hyland’s (2005b) and Hyland and Tse’s (2005a) frameworks to examine lexical and grammatical stance markers, has focused on the all sections of RAs. Regarding the disciplines in focus, a systematic random sampling was employed in which five dimensions, namely hard, soft, life, pure and applied have been taken into account. To give the study a diachronically comparative taste, the RAs published in the six leading journals in 1990 and 2010 were included. Accordingly, the following research questions are put forward:
1. What is the distribution of lexical and grammatical stance markers in subsections of 1990 RAs?
2. What is the distribution of lexical and grammatical stance markers in subsections of 2010 RAs?
3. Is there any difference in the use of stance markers over time and across disciplines?
METHOD
The Rationale for Discipline Selection
An inevitable dilemma in any discursive evaluation of RAs is the criteria based on which researchers would pick out the sample of their studies. In other words, which disciplines to include in RA studies has been a bone of contention for which there was no consensus for a long time. To resolve the issue, different procedures have been proposed to pave the ground for researchers to achieve a representative set of disciplines as their sample. One of the most well-established schemes for classifying disciplines is Biglan’s (1973) three-dimensional taxonomy of academic disciplines through which he has classified academic disciplines into three dichotomies, namely hard or soft, pure or applied, and life or nonlife. According to Alise and Teddlie (2010), “hard disciplines (e.g., chemistry) are those in which there is a high degree of paradigmatic consensus on the appropriate methods for investigating phenomena of interest” (p. 106). On the other hand, soft disciplines (e.g., education) are those where there is little paradigm consensus. In pure fields (e.g., geology) there is little concern for practical application. They are distinguished from applied ones (e.g., computer science) which are precisely about practical application of scientific concepts. The last distinction, life vs. nonlife, deals with “whether or not the discipline is concerned with living organisms” (Alise & Teddlie, 2010, p. 105). That is, the subject matter of life disciplines (e.g., microbiology), as the term itself implies, deals with any type of living thing, whereas the subject matter of nonlife disciplines (e.g., philosophy) revolves around nonorganic issues.
Taking into account these three dichotomies, the researchers have selected four disciplines of zoology, anthropology, horticulture, and education as the sample of study. Through selecting these disciplines, the authors have taken five dimensions, namely hard, soft, life, pure and applied into account and excluded non nonlife life dimension. In fact, in Biglan’s (1973) taxonomy, zoology is a hard/pure/life discipline as opposed to its soft counterpart i.e., anthropology which is a soft/pure/life discipline. Horticulture is a hard/applied/life discipline as opposed to education that is a soft/applied/life discipline. The rationale behind this selection draws on the researchers’ attempt to make the sample as representative as possible.
Corpus
For the purpose of the present study, 240 RAs were selected; 120 RAs from each of the time intervals mentioned. The selected RAs came from six leading and prestigious journals based on their impact factors cited on the SJR website (see www.scimagojr.com). Therefore, 60 RAs were selected from each discipline; 30 belonging to 1990 and 30 coming from 2010, yielding a total number of 1,270,021 words. As a result, the corpus was a representative sample of RAs belonging to four disciplines of anthropology, education, horticulture, and zoology in order to account for the totality of human knowledge spheres with respect to the life dimension leaving the nonlife dimension for studies to come.
Another fact regarding the sample of the present study is that, compared with other similar studies, it is unique in that it includes RAs published during two periods of time, namely 1990 and 2010.
Meanwhile, in order to come up with a detailed analysis, the RAs in the corpus were divided into their canonical subsections of Abstract, Introduction, Methodology, Results, and Discussion (Swales, 1990, 2004). These subsections have specific functions in reporting the scientific findings. The first section i.e., Abstract, “serves as the point of departure of most research articles and has progressively become a pivotal part in the writing of the research article” (Khedri, Heng, & Ebrahimi, 2013, p. 319). Regarding the Introduction section, Swales (1990) further explains that the authors try to situate their research in connection with other attempts in the literature, display the originality of the research, and finally demonstrate the main features of their study. The Methodology section of RAs is marked by references to procedures of inquiry leading to the ultimate results of the study based on the given data (Swales, 1990). Moreover, according to Swales (1990), the Results and the Discussion sections of RAs “mirror-image the Introduction section by moving from specific findings to wider implications” (p. 133). In other words, it is in these sections that the authors try to appeal to the members of their discourse community (Swales, 1990) and link their findings to those of others in the literature. Furthermore, as Abdi and Ahmadi (2015) state, the Discussion section “is the very part of the RA in which the researchers try to persuade their readers” (p. 11).
As mentioned above, one of the strong points of the present study is to take a diachronic perspective toward the genre of RA since genres are born, change, evolve, and decay (Bazerman, 1988). Therefore, a comparison is also made between the subsections of RAs over time to detect any possible changes in terms of the use of stance markers. This is done in order to come up with a general underlying pattern of the distribution of lexical and grammatical stance markers for the authors in the relevant disciplines especially for those who are new to their fields. As admittedly, being able to project oneself in the writing to stance behind the suggested propositions is a challenging part of the job (Hyland, 2009).
Data Collection Procedure
Having compiled the corpus, the four lexical subcategories of hedges, boosters, attitude markers, and self-mentions (Hyland, 2005a) as well as the grammatical evaluative that construction (Hyland & Tse, 2005a) were considered as representing the authorial stance in RAs. For the former, a list was borrowed from Hyland (2005a) and for the latter the study by Hyland and Tse (2005a) came to be the yardstick. As there is a growing consensus among scholars on the fuzzy nature of metadiscourse markers including stance markers (Ädel, 2006), the current research took a functional perspective while searching for targeted markers. Keeping that in mind, in order to yield reliable data, the number of relevant stance markers was counted by the researchers and two other colleagues and the ultimate number came from averaging out of the three independent frequency counts, and then, the relative frequency was calculated per 1000 words for each subcategory.
Data Analysis
In this study, in order to analyze the data so as to be able to address the research questions, the following statistical procedures were employed. As went above, a frequency count was carried out for the stance markers in different subsections of RAs. Then, in order to come up with a logical comparison, the frequency counts were changed into relative frequency per 1000 words. Finally, three rounds of chi-square were run to detect any differences in term of stance markers against disciplines, publication date, and RA subsections.
RESULTS
Results Related to the First Research Question
Regarding the first research question intended to investigate the distributional pattern of lexical and stance markers across the five subsections of RAs published in 1990, our study revealed the following results summarized in Table 1.
Table 1: The distribution of stance markers across subsections of RAs in 1990 (per 1000 words) |
|||||||
|
|
Abstract |
Introduction |
Methodology |
Results |
Discussion |
Total |
anthropology
|
Boosters |
8.4 |
18.7 |
17.8 |
13.5 |
16.0 |
14.9 |
Hedges |
29.3 |
42.2 |
39.2 |
37.1 |
40.9 |
37.7 |
|
Attitude mrks. |
6.8 |
7.2 |
7.6 |
8.3 |
9.6 |
7.9 |
|
Self-mentions |
13.6 |
16.8 |
12.8 |
24.6 |
17.5 |
17.1 |
|
That const. |
9.1 |
12.0 |
10.7 |
9.2 |
8.8 |
10.0 |
|
Total |
67.2 |
97.0 |
88.1 |
92.6 |
92.9 |
|
|
education
|
Boosters |
8.8 |
8.3 |
10.0 |
12.6 |
10.5 |
10.0 |
Hedges |
26.1 |
43.0 |
44.3 |
44.5 |
46.4 |
40.9 |
|
Attitude mrks. |
5.2 |
7.5 |
6.7 |
6.6 |
7.9 |
6.8 |
|
Self-mentions |
10.4 |
8.3 |
7.3 |
20.2 |
5.6 |
10.4 |
|
That const. |
7.8 |
8.3 |
7.1 |
7.6 |
10.0 |
8.2 |
|
Total |
58.2 |
75.4 |
75.5 |
91.5 |
80.4 |
|
|
horticulture
|
Boosters |
11.2 |
7.1 |
6.1 |
10.5 |
10.3 |
9.0 |
Hedges |
26.4 |
34.0 |
21.8 |
31.8 |
31.4 |
29.1 |
|
Attitude mrks. |
6.8 |
6.1 |
7.8 |
6.4 |
8.4 |
7.1 |
|
Self-mentions |
6.8 |
8.6 |
9.2 |
8.7 |
9.4 |
8.5 |
|
That const. |
6.9 |
7.0 |
6.2 |
9.2 |
9.0 |
7.6 |
|
Total |
58.1 |
62.7 |
51.1 |
66.6 |
68.5 |
|
|
zoology
|
Boosters |
7.1 |
10.6 |
10.1 |
9.2 |
11.2 |
9.6 |
Hedges |
27.3 |
34.3 |
26.9 |
39.4 |
34.8 |
32.6 |
|
Attitude mrks. |
7.0 |
7.7 |
5.5 |
5.5 |
6.0 |
6.3 |
|
Self-mentions |
7.6 |
9.5 |
11.8 |
6.9 |
6.2 |
8.4 |
|
That const. |
4.6 |
6.1 |
7.0 |
6.6 |
9.3 |
6.7 |
|
Total |
53.6 |
68.2 |
61.3 |
67.6 |
67.5 |
|
As Table 1 shows, in each academic discipline, a sizable number of words are employed to signpost the stance taken by the RA authors. On the whole, in 1990 RAs, hedges come first with regard to their relative frequency i.e., the relative frequency per 1000 words is 37.7, 40.9, 29.1, and 32.6 for anthropology, education, horticulture, and zoology respectively. This interestingly indicates that hedges are commonly used by the authors from different fields in order to avoid total commitment to their claims and arguments (Hyland, 2005a).
The second frequently applied stancetaking category is self-mentions for anthropology and education, and boosters for horticulture and zoology. In fact, this finding is understandably sensible as the first two disciplines belong to the soft extreme of the academic disciplines classification and the second two come from the hard extreme (Becher, 1989).
The third frequent category is boosters in anthropology and education, and self-mentions in horticulture and zoology. As mentioned earlier, boosters are used to express certainty and power of propositions. Therefore, due to the nature of soft disciplines, the authors have to put forward their claims in a way that persuasion is rhetorically guaranteed (Fu & Hyland, 2014).
The fourth stancetaking category based on frequency in Table 1 is evaluative that constructions (Hyland & Tse, 2005b). According to Hyland and Tse (2005b), this grammatical structure is widely used in academic writing by the authors to provide statements, comments, judgments, and evaluations.
The last stancetaking category in our analysis of 1990 RAs is attitude markers which are terms expressing writer’s appraisal of what is put forward in the discourse. The relative frequency for this stance marker is 7.9 and 6.8 for anthropology and education respectively, while it is 7.1 and 6.3 for horticulture and zoology.
Results Related to the Second Research Question
Having reviewed the RAs of 1990, the next step is to examine the RAs sampled from articles published in 2010 in order to care for the time factor as time interval is one of the variables of interest in the current study. Table 2 is about the distribution of stance markers and evaluative that in 2010 RAs.
Table 2: The distribution of stance markers across subsections of RAs in 2010 (per 1000 words) |
|||||||||
|
|
Abstract |
Introduction |
Methodology |
Results |
Discussion |
Total |
||
anthropology |
Boosters |
9.7 |
30.7 |
6.9 |
9.5 |
7.5 |
12.8 |
||
Hedges |
24.3 |
68.8 |
34.0 |
39.4 |
38.0 |
40.9 |
|||
Attitude mrks. |
6.1 |
6.6 |
7.7 |
7.3 |
6.9 |
6.9 |
|||
Self-mentions |
6.9 |
8.2 |
8.6 |
4.4 |
7.6 |
7.1 |
|||
That const. |
10.4 |
8.3 |
8.9 |
9.8 |
10.0 |
9.5 |
|||
Total |
57.4 |
122.6 |
66.2 |
70.5 |
70.0 |
|
|||
education
|
Boosters |
4.7 |
10.6 |
7.2 |
9.9 |
10.9 |
8.6 |
||
Hedges |
29.4 |
45.7 |
34.5 |
35.7 |
40.5 |
37.2 |
|||
Attitude mrks. |
6.9 |
7.6 |
7.4 |
6.3 |
8.7 |
7.4 |
|||
Self-mentions |
6.3 |
6.2 |
9.9 |
5.5 |
10.3 |
7.7 |
|||
That const. |
7.8 |
9.8 |
6.9 |
9.6 |
11.1 |
9.0 |
|||
Total |
55.1 |
79.9 |
65.9 |
67.1 |
81.4 |
|
|||
horticulture
|
Boosters |
14.9 |
5.4 |
7.9 |
11.2 |
13.8 |
10.6 |
||
Hedges |
25.4 |
25.9 |
16.4 |
25.7 |
25.0 |
23.7 |
|||
Attitude mrks. |
6.9 |
5.6 |
5.7 |
8.4 |
7.2 |
6.8 |
|||
Self-mentions |
6.7 |
7.6 |
6.6 |
8.9 |
5.2 |
7.0 |
|||
That const. |
6.5 |
5.0 |
6.5 |
5.9 |
6.6 |
6.1 |
|||
Total |
60.4 |
49.6 |
43.1 |
60.0 |
57.8 |
|
|||
zoology
|
Boosters |
12.1 |
7.2 |
6.4 |
9.2 |
10.4 |
9.1 |
||
Hedges |
32.5 |
33.8 |
6.4 |
28.9 |
35.9 |
27.5 |
|||
Attitude mrks. |
6.3 |
6.3 |
5.6 |
5.5 |
7.0 |
6.1 |
|||
Self-mentions |
11.2 |
8.7 |
15.8 |
7.5 |
9.5 |
10.6 |
|||
That const. |
6.3 |
6.1 |
5.3 |
4.6 |
8.8 |
6.2 |
|||
Total |
68.5 |
62.1 |
39.5 |
55.7 |
71.5 |
|
|||
Similar to 1990 RAs, in 2010 RAs, hedges rank first in their relative frequency compared to other markers of authorial stance in different subsections of RAs. However, those in the soft disciplines of anthropology and education outnumber the ones in the hard disciplines of horticulture and zoology. This can be justified with respect to the open argumentation and interpretation of soft disciplines as opposed to closed explanation of hard disciplines.
In terms of other makers, the picture gets complicated. For anthropology, boosters come second which shows that authors writing in this discipline try to strike a balance between uncertainty and certainty. For education, that construction ranks second in the list of stance markers as the discipline in which the grammatical marker has been frequently used. Boosters come second in horticulture and in zoology self-mentions hold the second rank among the stance markers. The third commonly applied stance marker is that construction, boosters, self-mentions, and boosters in anthropology, education, horticulture, and zoology respectively. The fourth frequently used stancetaking feature is self-mentions in anthropology and education, attitude markers in horticulture, and that construction in zoology. And finally, attitude markers rank last in anthropology, education, and zoology with that construction coming last in horticulture.
Results Related to the Third Research Question
In order to account for the messy picture which came about in 2010 RAs, Table 3 presents the total relative frequency of lexical and grammatical stance markers for the four disciplines surveyed in the present study.
Table 3: Stance features by discipline (per 1,000 words) |
||||||
Year |
Features |
Anthropology |
Education |
Horticulture |
Zoology |
Total |
1990 |
Lexical |
97 |
85.0 |
67.4 |
71.1 |
80.1 |
Grammatical |
49.9 |
40.8 |
38.2 |
33.5 |
40.6 |
|
Total |
146.9 |
125.8 |
105.6 |
104.6 |
|
|
2010 |
Lexical |
84.8 |
76.0 |
60.0 |
66.5 |
71.8 |
Grammatical |
47.4 |
45.1 |
30.5 |
31.1 |
38.5 |
|
Total |
132.2 |
121.1 |
90.5 |
97.6 |
|
As Table 3 shows, there is a discernible pattern in the use of stance markers within the disciplines themselves and across the sampling time intervals. The numbers clearly indicate that the use of such markers has decreased over time in general with one exception. In education, the frequency of that construction is anomalous compared to other markers with a rise from 40.8 to 45.1 moving from RAs in 1990 to those in 2010.
These are what we have on the surface of these number distributions. It would be logical to look at the statistical analysis of the results in order to detect any possible statistically significant difference. Table 4 shows the chi-square test results for the filed and recency variables.
Table 4: The results of chi-square tests |
|
|
Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) |
Field*Stance Markers |
.897 |
Recency*Stance Markers |
.659 |
Subsections*Stance Markers |
.000 |
As can be observed in Table 4, despite the marked differences between the frequencies of the stance markers with respect to the sampled fields or disciplines, recency of the publication (1990 vs. 2010), and subsections under study (Abstract, Introduction, Methodology, Results, and Discussion), just the difference between stance markers across subsections is statistically significant. In terms of the other two variables, no such significant difference was observed.
DISCUSSION
The results presented in the previous section came up with a clear picture of the distribution of lexical and grammatical stance markers across the five subsections of RAs in anthropology, education, horticulture, and zoology over time.
Regarding the distribution of stance markers across the canonical subsections of RAs in 1990, there was an uneven pattern. In anthropology and education, more stance markers came in the Results and Discussion sections of RAs, as in soft disciplines these sections are writer-oriented and sites for rhetorical argumentation and persuasion (Swales, 1990). Introduction comes next in which, using stance markers, writers try to establish themselves, their research, and the research of others. Therefore, after abstract, this section is the more reasonable place to make use of stancetaking by the authors by which they attempt to foreground their positionality (Jaffe, 2009) with regard to their research and that of others. Regarding the abstract, the results of the present study are in line with Gillaerts and Van de Velde (2010) in which they found a substantial number of words devoted to hedging in the abstract section of RAs, although, as they argue, abstracts must be a place for boosting rather than hedging.
As Abdi (2011), along with others, argues academic writers in soft disciplines are more tenable to include softening terms due to the fluid nature of such fields. However, in hard sciences, the authors can’t be flexible enough in terms of the propositions they provide due to the rigid nature of such fields (Becher & Trowler, 2001).
With respect to horticulture and zoology, no matter how hard the discipline is, the authors have to present themselves in their texts through first person pronouns and the relevant possessive pronouns and adjectives. In other words, the writers make their presence visible by standing behind their statements or decide to withhold such commitments (Fu & Hyland, 2014).
Another visible pattern is the notable employment of lexical stance markers in soft disciplines of anthropology and education. This can be attributed to the open nature of such disciplines in which the argumentation and reasoning tend to be more interpretive (Becher & Trowler, 2001). With respect to these two disciplines, anthropology writers have used more stance markers than those of the education. This difference can further be ascribed to another layer of science classification in which anthropology is a pure field and education is an applied discipline. Pure fields tend to align themselves with evolutionary sciences compared to applied disciplines which tend to come closer to normal sciences (Kuhn, 1970).
The same justification holds true for the hard counterpart. As horticulture is an applied discipline, stance markers outnumber those of zoology. Of course, care must be exercised in order not to mix metaphors here. In other words, one should be careful not to confuse the life with the nonlife. The latter has been taken as a control variable in the sampling process.
One possible justification would be the nature of hard sciences which is in line with Hyland (2010) in which he states that markers used to indicate stance in soft sciences far exceed those in the hard sciences as there is a general consensus that discursive argumentation, persuasion, and interpretation would come about in soft sciences. In line with this reasoning, more stancetaking is rendered in the soft disciplines sampled in this study as the numbers verify this fact.
Interestingly, as we move to 2010 RAs, the same pattern holds true with a little decline in the number of words employed to signpost the writer-oriented stancetaking. This change in such cognitive-generic subsections of RAs (Abdi, 2011), especially in the number of stance markers, is attributable to the converging nature of human knowledge. In other words, academic people tend to move in the direction as they are little springs which connect together to make up the river of human knowledge (Toulman, 2003). Therefore, little by little, the conventions and assumptions of a discourse community are settled down and the members of those communities get at home with the resolved facts. This is also evident according to the number of attitude markers in a way that as we move further toward the hard extreme of the disciplines, the use of such markers declines.
From a diachronic point of view, in line with Gillaerts and Van de Velde (2010), and having in mind the findings of others (e.g, Abdi, 2011; Abdollahzadeh, 2011), it can be inarguably claimed that stancetaking is a variable of time both longitudinally and cross-sectionally. This goes back to the nature of specific disciplines and general orientations that the relevant authors might have (Becher & Trowler, 2001). Another important point is that stancetaking is realized based on the specific function of the particular article subsection (Gross & Chesley, 2012). Therefore, it should be borne in mind that time and article aspect can play a pivotal role in how stancetaking in done in academic writing.
Regarding the frequency of grammatical evaluative that construction and the lexical stance markers of hedges, boosters, self-mentions, and attitude-markers, as observed in Table 3, lexical markers enjoyed high amount of usage than the grammatical counterpart by which the authors thematize their assertions with the noun, verb, and adjective as the controling terms. Therefore, the results of the current study in a way confirms the findings by Hyland and Tse (2005a).
CONCLUSION AND IMPLICATIONS
This study aimed to investigate stancetaking in English RAs realized through lexical markers borrowed from Hyland (2005b) and grammatical that construction taken from Hyland and Tse (2005a) in 240 RAs belonging to anthropology, education, horticulture, and zoology. For each field, there were 60 RAs, half of which was sampled from 1990 and the other half belonged to 2010. The main intention was first to examine the distribution of stance markers across the canonical subsections of RAs i.e., Abstract, Introduction, Methodology, Results, and Discussion over time and across disciplines, second, to investigate any possible differences regarding markers of stance in the fields under investigation.
Generally speaking, the distributions reported in this study all help us to explain a common thread running through the fields under study in this research: writers, consciously or unconsciously, make use of stance markers to first reflect their voice and second take account of their own text and their readers.
On closer inspection, hedges ranked first among the stancetaking markers particularly in the Discussion section. From a metadiscourse marking perspective, this is in contrast to Abdollahzadeh’s (2011) study in which he reported that in the Discussion section of the RAs, attitude markers were frequently applied by the American writers compared to the Persian writers.
It goes without saying that the findings of such studies as the present one must be cautiously interpreted according to the filed(s) under investigation. As reported by McGrath and Kuteeva (2012), their sample of mathematics RAs showed lower number of hedges and attitude markers in comparison with disciplines in soft sciences and other fields belonging to hard sciences. This not only waves the caution flag of interdisciplinary variations but also the nuances that might exist among the disciplines belonging to the same knowledge area.
The present research does have some applicable messages for the readers. First and foremost, stancetaking is an indispensable part of RAs realized through lexical metadiscourse markers and grammatical that constructions (Hyland, 2005b; Hyland & Tse, 2005a) being widely applied to signal the positionality of the authors (Jaffe, 2009). Therefore, this area must capture the attention of syllabus designers in English for academic purposes (EAP) and academic writing instructors as such, since admittedly the most difficult fraction of academic writing is the proper use of stance markers by the students in order to put forward their own ideas and evaluate those of others (Neff, Dafouz, Herrere, Martines, & Rica, 2003). Second, the predominance of some stance markers such as hedges over other devices must send out the signal of paying more attention to those makers being rampant from a frequency point of view. In other words, those markers frequently applied compared to other categories must be the first candidate for explicit instruction. This finding is in agreement with Abdollahzadeh and Zolfaghari-Erdechi (2012) who concluded that hedges were used with more frequency in narrative than the argumentative samples of written texts.
Moreover, it should be borne in mind that grammatical stancetaking can complement that of lexical items. The implication is that different forms must be applied in order to accomplish similar functions. So, academic writing instruction can focus on providing students with guidelines on different aspects of such writing and particularly making use of different markers to deploy stancetaking in a research article (Hyland & Tse, 2005b).
Any research endeavor confronts certain limitations demonstrating the necessary evil for gradual progression of human knowledge. First, due to practicality issues, just four disciplines were selected which by no means guarantees their representativeness for other disciplines. Therefore, other similar fields must be investigated as well as more disciplines belonging to the same science division keeping in mind that more is not necessarily better. Second, less observed fields having escaped the attention of the researchers can be selected for investigation. Third, the corpus of this study included 240 papers. Other studies might be conducted with more RAs in order to yield more generalizable findings and come up with more reliable and valid results. Fourth, in order to have a big picture of the academic writing with respect to RAs, other similar studies might replicate the present study with qualitative inquiry. For instance, interested researchers can complement such studies as this one by applying interviews in order to generate a more comprehensive image of how and why authors make use of stance markers in their writing enterprise. This can illuminate the underexplored corners of writing practice. Moreover, from a cross-cultural point of view, it goes without saying that different cultures have different ways of pointing out their positions regarding any viewpoint (Jaffe, 2009). Hence, similar studies can be launched to examine the cultural nuances realized in the way that writers apply stancetaking markers in their academic writing venture. Furthermore, the disciplines in this study were limited to life disciplines. Other similar studies can be carried out to focus on nonlife disciplines.
Finally, the engagement aspect of interaction which was left untouched in this study can be thoroughly scrutinized in similar studies. This will make the subsequent relevant research more fine-grained. Interested researchers can choose this interactional aspect of writing in genres other than RAs such as academic textbooks in terms of disciplinary specificity (Jalilifar, Alipour, & Parsa, 2014) to shed more light on the way(s) writers try to make connections with their readers in their written products.