Document Type : Research Paper

Authors

1 Ph.D. Student of Department of Foreign Languages, Science and Research Branch, Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran

2 Professor of Department of Psychology, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran

3 Professor of Department of TEFL, Islamic Azad University, Garmsar Branch, Tehran, Iran

Abstract

Attention plays a vital role in education. Children who have attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) suffer from impairing levels of inattention. ADHD is a relatively common childhood disorder (Scahill & Schwab-Stone, 2000), which, if left untreated, results in adverse consequences (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). This necessitates employing attention-training methods, such as neurofeedback training (NFT). But although conventional, NFT is expensive and time-consuming; therefore, the need for finding other methods is felt. The total physical response (TPR) method can provide a suitable venue for teaching young learners with ADHD (Nunan, 2011). Hence, this study was conducted to investigate the comparative effects of NFT and TPR on ADHD young learners’ attention. To do so, 16 students with ADHD were selected from a school in Shahryar. They were randomly assigned to NFT and TPR groups, receiving these treatments for twenty sessions, respectively. The Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test (IVA+Plus) was used as the pre- and post-test to measure full, auditory, and visual attention. To answer the research questions investigating the comparative effects of NFT and TPR on ADHD young learners’ attention, non-parametric one-way Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted. Moreover, the Wilcoxon Signed Rank test was used to explore within-group differences. The results indicated significant improvements for both groups. Nevertheless, NFT was found to be more effective regarding full and auditory attention. Concerning visual attention, both treatments were similarly effective. The findings suggest both treatments can improve participants’ attention. The study has implications for education by shedding light on attention-training methods.

Keywords

Main Subjects

INTRODUCTION

Regulating attention and behavior in agreement with instructed or internal goals is one of the most remarkable capacities of humans (Rico-Picó et al., 2021). Attention is closely related to memory, and goal-driven, self-regulated behavior, in view of its role in executive control (Rueda et al., 2021). Moreover, although there are studies suggesting the possibility of some kind of learning without attention (e.g., Hochstein & Pavlovskaya, 2020), they are mostly related to perceptual learning and not higher-order learning. Hence, education should aim at boosting attention.

            Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder, which is defined by impairing levels of inattention (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). It “is a relatively common [emphasis added] condition of childhood onset” (Scahill & Schwab-Stone, 2000, p. 541). The prevalence of ADHD in Iran varies across different studies. In school-aged children, it ranges from 5.03% to 29% and 2.3% to 15% for boys and girls, respectively (Hakim Shooshtari et al., 2021). Considering the high prevalence of ADHD in children in Iran (Darabi et al., 2022) and the adverse effects of untreated cases of this disorder, such as impairments in social communication, functional restrictions of effective communication, social participation, or academic achievement (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), measures should be taken to improve attention in people with attention deficit.

            Neurofeedback training (NFT) is a known cognitive training method for promoting attention, and its efficacy has been supported by many studies (e.g., Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2019; Ghadamgahi Sani et al., 2022; Riesco-Matías et al., 2021). Yet, NFT has been criticized on several accounts, such as being inefficient (Rahmani, et al., 2022), time-consuming, and expensive (Sho’ouri, 2021). Since there are studies indicating an association between socioeconomic disadvantage and the risk of ADHD (Keilow et al., 2020), parents of children with ADHD usually cannot afford its expenses. Therefore, the need for finding other methods for boosting attention is felt. 

            According to Nunan (2011), among different language teaching methods, total physical response (TPR) can provide a suitable venue for teaching young learners with ADHD. Yet, few studies have investigated the effect of TPR on attention. Furthermore, (to the author’s knowledge) even those few studies are mostly not empirical, and they have just concluded through reasoning that TPR can improve attention (e.g., Kováčiková & Reid, 2018). There are experimental studies on the effect of TPR on ADHD, but (to the author’s knowledge) they fall into two categories. That is, either they are not concerned directly with the effect of TPR on attention (e.g., Pramesti, 2021), or they have not measured attention objectively (De La Cruz et al., 2020).

            The reason for juxtaposing TPR and NFT is that there are studies indicating that both methods have the potential to increase attention (Kováčiková & Reid, 2018; Riesco-Matías et al., 2021). Moreover, since NFT is a known attention-training technique, the effects of TPR on attention can be compared with the effects of NFT on attention. One of the advantages of comparative studies is that identifying an improvement potential may be easier when there is a reference level (Ellingsen et al., 2009). Therefore, this study has been carried out to investigate the comparative effects of NFT and TPR on young ADHD learners’ attention. To do so, 16 schoolgirls with ADHD took part in this study. They were randomly divided into two groups, receiving NFT and TPR treatments, respectively, for twenty sessions. Full attention, auditory attention, and visual attention were measured by the IVA+Plus test prior to and after treatments. Non-parametric ANCOVA and the Wilcoxon Signed Rank test were used to answer the research questions.

 

LITERATURE REVIEW

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

According to American Psychiatric Association (2013), ADHD is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by impairing levels of inattention, disorganization, and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity. Inattention and disorganization include inability to stay on task, appearing not to listen, and losing materials at levels not consistent with age or developmental level. Hyperactivity-impulsivity includes overactivity, constant moving, not being able to stay seated, barging into other people’s activities, and not being able to wait. If left untreated, ADHD may bring about impairments in social communication, functional limitations of effective communication, social participation, or academic achievement (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

            Such impaired functioning may, in part, be due to inattentiveness (Sroubek et al., 2013). According to American Psychiatric Association (2013), “ADHD occurs in most cultures in about 5% of children” (p. 61). It should be mentioned that “a huge variability in prevalence rates is detected among different studies, ranging from as low as 1% to as high as nearly 20% among school-age children” (Ercan et al., 2015, pp. 1145–1146). This variation is reported not only across different studies in the world but also in Iran. According to Hakim Shooshtari et al. (2021), the reported ADHD prevalence among school-aged children in Iran fluctuates between 5.03% and 29% for boys and 2.3% to 15% for girls.

            As it can be observed, ADHD has inflicted a large population in the world, hence its significance. Many effective treatments are available for ADHD, including pharmacological, nonpharmacological, and multimodal treatments (Harpin et al., 2016). Recognition of parents’ attitudes towards different treatment types is important because it may improve our understanding of the type of therapeutic decisions they make (Jiang et al., 2014). On the whole, “[a]dverse effects, concerns about stigmatization, and the child’s dislike of taking pills, all contribute to parents’ decisions to discontinue medication even when the child shows symptomatic benefit” (Charach et al., 2006, p. 75). Therefore, many parents would rather not use any form of medication for the treatment of their children’s ADHD (Charach et al., 2006). Consequently, for these children, alternative treatment methods must be adopted. One of the nonpharmacological cognitive methods for alleviating symptoms of ADHD, which is gaining increasing attention, is neurofeedback (Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2019).

 

Neurofeedback Training (NFT)

Historical Background of Neurofeedback

In 1875, the electrical activity of the brain was discovered by Richard Caton, the British physician and physiologist (Caton, 1875). Later, in 1924, this electrical activity was recorded by Hans Berger, the German psychiatrist. Following the term electrocardiogram, Berger coined the term electroencephalogram (EEG) to refer to it (Berger, 1929). These, together with Pavlov’s conditioning and Skinner’s operant conditioning, were inspirations for the development of biofeedback and EEG biofeedback, also known as neurofeedback.

            In 1962, Joe Kamiya found out that some subjects would learn through operant conditioning not only to determine whether they were in the alpha state but also to use feedback to produce this state (Kamiya, 1962). In the same vein, Sterman and Wyrwicka (1967) showed that it was possible to teach cats to produce sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) through operant conditioning (i.e., by providing food rewards for the occurrence of SMR). Later, Sterman and Friar (1972) were successfully able to suppress seizures in an epileptic patient following SMR EEG feedback training- now known as neurofeedback or EEG biofeedback.

 

Neurofeedback Definition and Priming

Neurofeedback is a neural stimulation technique for changing brain behavior (Mirifar et al., 2022). In other words, it is an electroencephalographic biofeedback technique for training individuals to change their brain activity (Thompson & Thompson, 2021). “Biofeedback is a behavioral training program wherein individuals learn to control their own autonomic nervous system, thereby attaining the ability to control such bodily functions as heart rate, blood pressure, skin temperature, or muscle relaxation” (Huber & Gillaspy, 2011, p. 38). When biofeedback, a system operating based on operant conditioning and feedback, is used for controlling brainwaves, it is referred to as EEG biofeedback or neurofeedback (Thompson & Thompson, 2021). NFT can be used for treating or alleviating different diseases and disorders, such as ADHD, seizure disorders, depression, anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder (Thompson & Thompson, 2021).

            Enriquez-Geppert et al. (2017) have summarized the basic setup of a brain-computer interface for neurofeedback (see Figure 1). According to them, first, the EEG gets recorded. Then, the data undergo processing, which refers to artifact detection, rejection, or correction. Afterwards, feature generation and extraction, and computation of the data will take place. Finally, during the last stage, which closes the feedback loop, the presentation of the feedback signal will happen. At this stage, participants try to learn to utilize the feedback signal to change their brain activity that complies with instructions.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Figure 1. The basic setup of a brain-computer interface for neurofeedback (Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2017, p. 151)

 

During a typical NFT session, one or more electrodes are positioned on the scalp, and on the earlobes usually one or two are placed. No electrical current is put into the brain (Hammond, 2011). Through using electrodes, which are attached to the scalp, the electrical activity of the brain is sent to an electroencephalograph and a computer. These data are then processed and shown to the trainee in a format similar to that of a video game (Butnik, 2005). In some versions, the trainee sits in front of a computer monitor and tries to play a game. The neurofeedback device has been devised in such a way that if, for instance, the trainee increases the desired brainwave activity or decreases the undesired one, the game continues. But, the game stops if the trainee’s mind begins to wander (Amen, 2015).

     In all cases, the trainees can have knowledge of their own brainwave, which is “visually displayed to them, typically in the form of a bar graph whose height is proportional to the real-time EEG amplitude and which fluctuates accordingly” (Bagdasaryan & Le Van Quyen, 2013, p. 2). Through operant conditioning, trainees can “learn to manipulate this visual feedback, increasing/decreasing it to a predefined threshold level, with a reward when amplification/suppression to this threshold is achieved” (Bagdasaryan & Le Van Quyen, 2013, pp. 2–3). 

     There are different protocols for NFT, and none of them is considered a gold standard (Kropotov, 2016). In the context of NFT, the term protocol refers to the places of electrodes and frequencies which will be rewarded and inhibited (Ogrim & Hestad, 2013). People with ADHD usually have greater rates of slower EEG activity (delta, theta, or even alpha) in comparison to faster beta activity; therefore, most studies on ADHD have used protocols directed toward decreasing the large quantity of slow frequencies while enhancing the magnitude of fast frequencies (Yucha & Montgomery, 2008). Three standard NFT protocols are theta/beta (theta suppression/beta enhancement), sensorimotor rhythm (SMR), and slow cortical potential (SCP) (Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2019).

 

Total Physical Response (TPR)

TPR is a language teaching method established by James Asher, a professor of psychology at San Jose State University, California. This method tries to teach a foreign language by having students listen to commands in a foreign language and immediately obey them by performing physical actions (Asher, 1969). It is associated with the natural approach to language learning because it has some similarities to how children learn their first language (Asher, 1969). According to Asher et al. (1974), three key ideas underlying TPR are as follows: (1) Most linguistic items can be taught through imperatives. And if this technique is used creatively by teachers, it will be motivating to students for a long-term training program, (2) Listening skill can help other skills to develop. There is a large        magnitude of transfer from listening to other skills, (3) Language learning is most optimally achieved through the engaging students’ bodies when commands are set. This approach will help the internalization of language in chunks rather than word by word.

            TPR is based on the stimulus-response pattern of behaviorism. Because in this method, the learners’ correct response to a teacher’s verbal stimulus results in praise (Wheeler, 2013). TPR has put lots of emphasis on the elicitation of physical responses from the learners, such as moving, reaching, jumping, grabbing, etc. Therefore, this method puts emphasis on right-brain learning. Asher believes that motor activity is a right-brain function, and it should happen before left-brain language
processing (Asher, 1982). Moreover, TPR is a good example of embodied cognition in language acquisition (Zhou, 2021). Embodied cognition is a learning theory suggesting that the incorporation of knowledge into the body’s sensorimotor system makes learning and understanding abstract concepts easier (Chettaoui, et al., 2022).

            TPR is appropriate for children with ADHD (Nunan, 2011) because it reduces anxiety. Anxiety disorder is one of ADHD’s common comorbidities (Quenneville et al., 2022). TPR reduces anxiety of children with ADHD by not forcing them to speak when they are not ready. According to Asher (1969), “[i]f the student achieves a high level of listening fluency then the transition to speaking may be graceful and non-stressful” (Asher, 1969, pp. 16–17). TPR also reduces anxiety by employing the element of fun because, according to Asher (1982), it encourages commands that are zany, playful, bizarre, and crazy. Moreover, physical activity reduces anxiety (Carter et al., 2021).

            Furthermore, it is appropriate because children have a limited attention span, and this limited span gets worse in the case of children with ADHD (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). To solve this problem, Nunan (2011) has recommended changing activities so that learners will not lose interest. In Nunan’s (2011) view, TPR activities can provide a suitable venue for realizing such an aim because they are motivating and suit different learning styles, such as visual, auditory, and kinesthetic. Also, TPR is appropriate for children with ADHD because it caters to their strong bodily-kinesthetic intelligence because this method “relies on the use of bodily-kinesthetic intelligence” (Maftoon & Najafi Sarem, 2012, p. 1235). 

 

PURPOSE OF THE STUDY

Considering the paramount importance of attention in education, this research was conducted to investigate the comparative effects of NFT and TPR on ADHD young learners’ attention. The research questions (RQs) in this study, aim at exploring between-groups comparisons (i.e., whether NFT or TPR is more efficient at boosting full attention, auditory attention, and visual attention, respectively). In this respect, the following RQs were posed:

(1) Is there any significant difference between the effects of NFT and TPR on ADHD young learners’ full attention?

(2) Is there any significant difference between the effects of NFT and TPR on ADHD young learners’ auditory attention?

(3) Is there any significant difference between the effects of NFT and TPR on ADHD young learners’ visual attention?      

 

METHOD

Participants

Sixteen children with ADHD aged 7 to 12 were enrolled to attend this study by using a convenience sampling procedure. The participants were selected from a school in Shahryar, from beginner English learners suspected to have ADHD. They were all Iranian schoolgirls whose mother tongues were Persian. The participants were randomly assigned to two equal groups of A and B, receiving NFT and TPR for twenty sessions, respectively. The schematic representation of the design of the study is like the following:

Group A → IVA+Plus (pretest) → NFT treatment → IVA+Plus (posttest)
Group B → IVA+Plus (pretest) → TPR treatment → IVA+Plus (posttest)  

 

 

Instrumentation

Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test (IVA+Plus)

IVA+Plus is a test designed by Sandford and Turner (1994). It has been used for two purposes in this study: a) as one of the screening devices assisting in the diagnosis of ADHD, and b) to measure participants’ attention. It should be mentioned that this test can be used for both purposes (Sandford & Turner, 1994; Wang, et al., 2021). IVA+Plus falls under the category of Continuous Performance Tests (CPTs). It is a computerized test of attention in which the testees should respond to 500 intermixed auditory and visual stimuli. Stimuli are auditory and visual 1 or 2 (i.e., testees would see or hear a 1 or a 2). The task of the testees is to click the mouse when they see number 1 or hear the word one and to refrain from clicking when they see number 2 or hear the word two (IVA+Plus, n.d.). This test has two composite scores (i.e., Attention Quotient and Response Control). The attention quotient is subdivided into auditory and visual attention. And auditory and visual attention are subdivided into vigilance, focus, and speed (Tinius, 2003). Figure 2 represents different attention scales measured by the IVA test.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Figure 2. Descriptions of scales measured in the IVA CPT (Tinius, 2003, p. 452)

 

Since this study is concerned just with the attention quotient and its subdivisions (i.e., auditory and visual attention), the sub-subsections (i.e., vigilance, focus, and speed) and the response control scores have not been discussed. It should be mentioned that IVA+Plus “yields an extremely large number of possible scores” (Strauss, et al., Spreen, 2006, p. 576). Therefore, different researchers use just part of the test. In this study, IVA+Plus was used for measuring ADHD young learners’ attention before and after NFT and TPR treatments. The assessments were carried out at intervals of 2–5 days before and after interventions. “Attention refers to the process of selecting task-relevant stimuli and inhibiting task-irrelevant distractors, and it helps us to allocate the mental resources involved in a vast number of simultaneous inputs from visual, auditory and other sensory modalities” (Wu, et al., 2021, p. 1). Therefore, visual attention and auditory attention refer to the ability to select simultaneous inputs from visual and auditory modalities, respectively.

 

Data Collection Procedure

The first criteria for selecting participants were teachers’ and parents’ reports. In these reports, teachers and parents were asked whether they thought their children were suffering from ADHD and whether they thought they had nearly no command of English. Based on these reports, 53 students were selected. The selected students were interviewed by the researcher to make sure that they nearly had no command of English. It should be mentioned that the reports were just used for screening purposes, although some parents claimed that their children had already been diagnosed with ADHD.   

            Subsequently, the selected students sat for the IVA+Plus test to help with the initial diagnosis of ADHD. Based on the results of this test, 23 students with an initial diagnosis of ADHD were selected. Finally, to ascertain the diagnosis of ADHD, the selected students were referred to a psychiatrist. Based on the psychiatrist’s diagnosis and as confirmed by administering the structured clinical interview for American Psychiatric Association (2013), 16 students were recognized to be suffering from ADHD. The selected students were in stable clinical condition at the time of enrolment, as recognized by the psychiatrist. Written, informed-consents for attending the research were obtained from the participants’ parents or their guardians. The participants were randomly divided into groups A and B. Group A in this study received 20 sessions of one-hour NFT, and group B had 20 sessions of one-hour TPR. NFT and TPR sessions were held three times a week in the morning before noon.  

          During NFT sessions, a trained operator, after attaching some electrodes to each child’s head, briefed them in Persian that they have to focus their attention while watching a computer game. The operator explained that, by focusing their attention, they have to keep the green bar (representing the desired brainwave) up and the red one (representing the undesired one) down; otherwise, the game stops, and they have to attract their attention again to make the game restart. The sessions were held for each participant separately. The neurofeedback protocol used for ADHD in this study was a “beta-training protocol with Fz–Cz placement of electrodes and the NF parameter as the ratio of EEG power in the 13- to 21-hz band and EEG power in the 4- to 12-hz frequency band” (Kropotov, 2016, p. 258).

Following Professor Asher’s (personal communication, January 10, 2019) recommendation, Nancy Márquez’s (2011) book was used for TPR classes. Care was taken to make commands as funny as possible. Aside from using the TPR’s major technique, namely commands (Asher, 1969; 1982; Asher et al., 1974), some classic TPR activities such as Simon says were used. In this game, students are supposed to perform only those orders that have been preceded by the clause Simon says. To make the game more interesting and students more motivated, the researcher changed it slightly. In each round, one of the students was appointed boss. Afterward, her name was used instead of Simon (e.g., Setayesh says). Then, the teacher or the student herself, if willing, would give orders. Following the game’s rule, students were supposed to jump up high if the order was Setayesh says, jump up high and make no movement if the order was just Jump up high.

Some of the other activities used were carrying out commands with music songs. The other group of activities involved a whiteboard. Here, participants were asked to carry out drawing orders (e.g., Draw a house!) or to draw something on an incomplete drawing while being blindfolded. For example, the teacher drew a circle as an incomplete face and asked students to complete the drawing by issuing orders (e.g., Draw the hair!). In the same vein, the other group of activities involved using a noticeboard. Here, participants were asked to carry out sticking orders or to stick something on an incomplete drawing while being blindfolded (e.g., Stick the nose!). Other activities involved performing commands involving the use of realia (e.g., ball, teddy bear, book, notebook, pen, pencil, lunch box, school bag, school plastic cup, etc.). Moreover, as students made progress, an attempt was made to use more action sequences comprising three or more connected commands (e.g., Take a pin. Pick up a nest. Stick the nest on the tree.). Furthermore, new (unheard-of) commands were issued occasionally. Each session started by reviewing commands issued in the previous session or sessions.

 

RESULTS

Since the two samples in this study were selected from among ADHD young learners whose population does not meet the normality assumption, and due to the small sample sizes in this study, all RQs were analyzed through nonparametric tests.

 

Between-Groups Comparisons

To answer research questions in this study, three non-parametric one-way ANCOVA were conducted to compare the mean of the NFT group with that of the TPR group on full, auditory, and visual attention, respectively. The results were obtained after controlling for the effect of the pretest on full, auditory, and visual attention, respectively. As displayed in Table 1, the NFT group had a higher mean than that of the TPR group on the posttest of full attention and auditory attention after controlling for the effect of these items on the pretest. And as regards visual attention, an opposite scenario is observed.

 

Table 1: Descriptive statistics; posttest of full, auditory, and visual attention     by groups with pretest

Groups

 

Mean

Std. Error

 

95% Confidence Interval

Lower Bound

Upper Bound

NFT Full Attention

103.319

2.206

98.554

108.084

TPR Full Attention

91.806

2.206

87.041

96.571

NFT Auda Attention

108.774

2.614

103.126

114.421

TPR Auda Attention

82.726

2.614

77.079

88.374

NFT Visb Attention

97.412

3.395

90.076

104.747

TPR Visb attention

102.088

3.395

94.753

109.424

  1. Auditory
  2. Visual

 

Table 2 shows the results obtained from non-parametric ANCOVA. The results (F (1, 14) = 11.99, p < .050) and (F (1, 14) = 8.67, p < .050) indicated that the NFT group significantly had a higher mean on full attention and auditory attention than those of the TPR group. However, concerning visual attention, the results (F (1, 14) = .119, p > .050) showed that there was no significant difference between NFT and TPR groups’ means on the posttest of visual attention after controlling for the effect of the pretest.

 

Table 2: Nonparametric analysis of covariance; posttest of full, auditory, and visual attention by groups with pretest

Attention Type

F

DFH

DFE

p Value

Full Attention

11.997

1

14

.004

Auda Attention

8.670

1

14

.011

Visb Attention

0.119

 1

14

.735

  1. Auditory
  2. Visual

 

Thus, the results indicated that while the NFT group had significantly higher mean scores on full attention and auditory attention than those of the TPR group, no significant difference was observed between the two groups regarding visual attention.      

 

Within-Group Analyses

While between-groups analyses indicated significant differences between NFT and TPR groups concerning full attention and auditory attention, other analyses are needed to investigate differences occurring within each of these groups from the pre- to post-test. Put simply, within-group analyses are needed to find out the efficacy of NFT and TPR in boosting full attention, auditory attention, and visual attention, respectively. To do so, Wilcoxon Signed Rank tests were conducted to compare each group’s medians on full, auditory, and visual attention sections of the pretest with those of the posttest. As displayed in Table 3, the NFT and TPR groups’ median scores on the posttests of full, auditory, and visual attention were higher than those on the pretests. Table 4 displays the results of the Wilcoxon Signed Rank test. 

 

 

 

  Group

Attention Type

Median

N

Mean Ranks

Sum of Ranks

 

 

 

   NFT

 Full Attention

 Pre Full = 68.50

 Post Full = 103.00

 Negative Ranks

8

4.50

36.00

 Positive Ranks

0

.00

.00

 Ties

0

 

 

 Total

8

 

 

Auda Attention

Pre Auda = 59.50

 Post Auda = 101.00

Negative Ranks

8e

4.50

36.00

Positive Ranks

0c

.00

.00

Ties

0d

 

 

 Total

8

 

 

Visb Attention

 

Pre Visb = 81.50

Post Visb = 103.50

 

Negative Ranks

8

4.50

36.00

Positive Ranks

0

.00

.00

Ties

0

 

 

 Total

8

 

 

 

 

 

   TPR

 

Full Attention

Pre Full = 84.00

 Post Full = 97.00

Negative Ranks

8

4.50

36.00

Positive Ranks

0c

.00

.00

Ties

0d

 

 

 Total

8

 

 

Auda Attention

Pre Auda = 86.50

Post Auda = 92.00

 

 Negative Ranks

7

4.57

32.00

Positive Ranks

1c

4.00

4.00

Ties

0d

 

 

 Total

8

 

 

Visb Attention

Pre Visb = 76.50

Post Visb = 101.00

 

 Negative Ranks

8

4.50

36.00

Positive Ranks

0c

.00

.00

Ties

0d

 

 

 Total

8

 

 

Table 3: Median scores and mean ranks on pretest and posttest of full, auditory, and visual attention by groups

  1. Auditory
  2. Visual

 

Concerning the NFT group, the results (Z = -2.52, p < .05, r = .999 representing a large effect size) showed that this group had a significantly higher median score on the posttest of full attention than that of the pretest. The other results (Z = -2.52, p < .05, r = .99 representing a large effect size) depicted that the NFT group had a significantly higher median score on the posttest of auditory attention than that of the pretest. And finally, the results (Z = -2.52, p < .05, r = .99 representing a large effect size) demonstrated that the NFT group had a significantly higher median score on the posttest of visual attention than that of the pretest.

            With respect to the TPR group, the results (Z = -2.52, p < .05, r = .99 representing a large effect size) showed that the TPR group had a significantly higher median score on the posttest of full attention than that of the pretest. The other results (Z = -1.97, p < .05, r = .778 representing a large effect size) indicated that the TPR group had a significantly higher median score on the posttest of auditory attention than that of the pretest. And finally, the results (Z = -2.52, p < .05, r = .99 representing a large effect size) indicated that the TPR group had a significantly higher median score on the posttest of visual attention than that of the pretest.

 

Table 4: Wilcoxon Signed Rank test; pretest and posttest of full, auditory, and visual attention by groups

Group

Attention Type

Z

Pretest- Posttest

 

 

 

NFT

NFT Full Attention

Z

-2.527

 

Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed)

.012

NFT Auda Attention

Z

-2.521

 

Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed)

.012

NFT Visb Attention

Z

-2.521

 

Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed)

.012

 

 

TPR

TPR Full Attention

Z

-2.521

 

Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed)

.012

TPR Auda Attention

Z

-1.975

 

Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed)

.048

TPR Visb Attention

Z

-2.521

 

Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed)

.012

  1. Auditory
  2. Visual

 

Overall, both NFT and TPR groups show significant improvements in their full attention, auditory attention, and visual attention from the pretest to the posttest of IVA+Plus.

 

DISCUSSION

The major hypothesis in this research was that, similar to NFT, the TPR method of language teaching has the potential to improve attention. The results indicated significant improvements for both NFT and TPR groups in their full attention, auditory attention, and visual attention. Nevertheless, NFT was found to be a more effective treatment concerning full attention and auditory attention. And as regards visual attention, both treatments were found to be similarly effective. In this section, first, a general discussion and then a specific discussion of RQs have been provided.

            Generally, it can be argued that the obtained results can be supported, in part, by noting that both NFT and TPR are based on the same learning theory (i.e., behaviorism). NFT is based on reinforcement learning, namely operant conditioning (Mirifar et al., 2022), and TPR is also based on the stimulus-response pattern (Wheeler, 2013) and reinforcement learning of operant conditioning (Asher, 1966). Operant conditioning can be a very helpful teaching model for children with ADHD (Wender & Tomb, 2017). There are many pieces of evidence supporting the efficacy of operant conditioning for improving undesirable behaviors in children with ADHD (e.g., De Meyer et al., 2019; Ryan & McDougall, 2009). Also, theoretically, it can be argued that while both NFT and TPR are based on behaviorism (Mirifar et al., 2022; Wheeler, 2013), attention itself may include habit learning mechanisms (Seger, 2018). According to Seger (2018), the habitual nature of attention can be shown by the fMRI and EEG studies conducted by Anderson et al. (2014) and Luque et al. (2017), respectively. Therefore, if attention can be construed to be habitual, then it can be improved through methods based on behaviorism such as NFT and TPR.

            With respect to RQs 1-3, it can be argued that the supremacy of NFT over TPR as regards full attention and auditory attention is not unexpected. It should be noted that there is not only a lack of comparative studies comparing the efficacy of NFT and TPR regarding attention, but also there are a few empirical studies objectively measuring the effects of TPR on attention. Nevertheless, the obtained results are expected because there are many studies supporting the efficacy of NFT for improving ADHD young learners’ attention (e.g., Louthrenoo et al., 2022; Riesco-Matías et al., 2021). Moreover, there are studies indicating the efficacy of NFT to improve auditory attention (e.g., Ghaziri et al., 2013; Hajehforoush et al., 2018).

            The other reason for the supremacy of NFT can be attributed to the
individualistic nature of this method as opposed to the collective nature of TPR. That is, while the behavior of other students did not affect the behavior of each recipient of the NFT treatment, the behavior of each student in the TPR group was influenced by the behavior of other students. It should be noted that undesirable behaviors are usually observed among children with ADHD. Peer relationships among these children are often disrupted by peer rejection, negligence, or teasing (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). They may reveal substantial changes in mood within the very day or display excessive anger and irritability; they may have a tantrum (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

            Overall, since they suffer from diminished self-control, they may display difficult-to-manage behavior (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Therefore, behaviors such as occasional temper tantrums, fierce rivalry, hostility, bullying, and rows among participants (i.e., verbal and physical aggression) frequently slowed down the speed of learning in the collective TPR group, issues not observed in the individualistic NFT group. Consequently, the TPR teacher had to tackle all these issues. And all these issues may explain, at least in part, the supremacy of NFT over TPR with respect to full attention and auditory attention.

            Moreover, as regards RQ 3, the findings can be justified because while NFT is a successful method in improving visual attention (e.g., Ghadamgahi Sani et al., 2022; Ghaziri et al., 2013), there are also many studies indicating that gesture improves visual attention (e.g., Araya et al., 2016; Hamilton, 2017; Wakefield et al., 2018). Therefore, since gesture is one of the main techniques used in the TPR method, the effectiveness of TPR for improving visual attention is expected.

            Furthermore, the findings are in line with one of the behavioral learning theories, which, according to Ryan and McDougall (2009), is suitable for children with ADHD, namely social learning theory and the benefits of observational learning proposed by Bandura (1977). According to Bandura (1977), when people are in any social group, models possessing engaging qualities are likely to attract greater attention than others. Therefore, the TPR teacher and students who carry out engaging orders can attract greater attention. Hence, it can be argued that while the collective nature of TPR sometimes deterred teaching and caused full attention and auditory attention in this group to lag behind those of the NFT group, it has, apparently, worked to the benefit of visual attention in the TPR group because of the benefits of observational learning. This issue can be supported by empirical studies supporting the positive effects of observational learning on visual attention (e.g., Koch et al., 2018; Yussen, 1974).

            With respect to NFT within-group analyses, it can be argued that since neurofeedback is based on operant conditioning (Mirifar et al., 2022; Thompson & Thompson, 2021), the findings of these analyses can be explained by the role of operant conditioning in improving attention. There are studies indicating that attention can be trained by operant conditioning. For example, Silverstein et al. (2001) found that an operant conditioning technique known as shaping can improve attention in severely ill schizophrenia patients. Also, there are studies indicating operant training of the auditory evoked potential in man (e.g., Finley & Johnson, 1983; Rosenfeld et al., 1969). Moreover, Price et al. (2016) found that visual attention can be trained based on operant conditioning of eye gaze. Their study is relevant to the findings of this research because in each NFT session, the participants are involved in a kind of human-computer interface in which the eye gaze plays a prominent role.

            Concerning TPR within-group analyses, it can be argued that since enactment and gesture are major techniques used in TPR, the findings of these analyses can be supported by studies indicating that these techniques lead to enhanced attention. In this regard, one can refer to studies conducted by Macedonia and Mueller (2016) and Wakefield et al. (2018). Moreover, the findings of this section can be further supported by the study conducted by Kováčiková and Reid (2018). Because their study indicated that TPR could be used as a mindfulness training tool to enhance mindful attention. Also, TPR’s potential to improve auditory attention can be supported by the findings of the study conducted by Nagels et al. (2018). They found that accompanying gestures with speech increases auditory cortex activation. In addition, TPR’s potential to improve visual attention can be supported by the findings of the study carried out by Araya et al. (2016) and Wakefield et al. (2018).

 

CONCLUSION AND IMPLICATIONS

This study aimed to investigate the comparative effects of NFT and TPR on ADHD young learners’ attention. The findings of this study indicated that both TPR and NFT can improve ADHD young learners’ attention, although NFT was found to be a more effective treatment as regards full attention and auditory attention.

            One of the pedagogical implications of this study would be employing TPR in the educational system not only for teaching language but also for boosting attention. The other implication would concern training teachers to employ this method in their classes. At the same time, the limitations of the study should be taken into account. Both convenience sampling and small sample sizes used in this study are the sources of threats to validity (McEwan, 2020). Also, physical and psychological fluctuations of the participants undergoing measurement (especially those of children) should be taken into notice. For instance, lack of sleep, observing/having a row prior to the test, physical pain at the time of the test, anxiety caused by entering new places, meeting new people, or observing new measurement devices, etc. should be considered.

            Moreover, it should be noted that the natures of the two treatments used in this study are different. That is, while NFT is an individualistic method, TPR is a collective method. The success or failure of collective methods depends on the participation of the whole group. In such methods, the unpunctuality of one student affects the whole class. The late attendance of some students in the TPR group slowed down the speed of learning. Furthermore, since undesirable behaviors are usually observed among most children with ADHD (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), difficult-to-manage behaviors frequently displayed among participants slowed down the speed of learning in the TPR group, an issue not observed in the NFT group.

            Considering the limitations of this study, further interdisciplinary research is needed to thoroughly investigate the comparative effects of TPR and NFT on attention. Such interdisciplinary research is needed because the third millennium “era has brought about a growing understanding of the inadequacy of the findings of every discipline claiming to be standing on its own like an isolated island providing omniscient knowledge” (Maftoon & Taie, 2016, p. 41). Leaning issues, once monopolized by the field of education, are now being tackled by diverse disciplines such as psychology, medicine, nutrition, biology, pharmacology, and sports. In line with this study, triangulation of this research with different participants, e.g., young learners without ADHD, is recommended. Also, future research can explore the comparative effects of NFT and other language teaching methods on attention. Moreover, they can investigate the effects of different language teaching strategies on attention. In the same vein, the role of different types of feedback in increasing attention can be investigated. Finally, the comparative effects of TPR and other psychological therapies improving cognitive processes can be explored. In this regard, it is recommended that future research investigate cognitive remediation therapy (CRT), as it is specifically designed to improve attention, memory, and other executive functions (Franza et al., 2018).

 

 

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

 

 

ORCID

Masumeh Taie

https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3933-8784

Reza Rostami

https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9318-108X

Massood Yazdanimoghaddam

https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1020-6569

 

Amen, D. G. (2015). Change your brain, change your life: The breakthrough program for conquering anxiety, depression, obsessiveness, lack of focus, anger, and memory problems. Harmony.  
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
Anderson, B. A., Laurent, P. A., & Yantis, S. (2014). Value-driven attentional
priority signals in human basal ganglia and visual cortex. Brain Research, 1587, 88–96. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.08.062   
Araya, R., Farsani, D., & Hernández, J. (2016). How to attract students’ visual attention. In K. Verbert, M. Sharples, & T. Klobučar (Eds.), Lecture notes in computer science: Vol. 9891. Adaptive and adaptable learning (pp. 30–41). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-45153-4
Asher, J. J. (1966). The learning strategy of the total physical response: A review. The Modern Language Journal, 50(2), 79–84. http://doi.org/10.2307/323182
Asher, J. J. (1969). The total physical response approach to second language
        learning. The Modern Language Journal, 53(1), 3–17. http://doi.org/10.2307/322091
Asher, J. J. (1982). Learning another language through actions: The complete teacher’s guidebook (2nd ed.). Sky Oaks Productions.
Asher, J. J., Kusudo, J. A., & de la Torre, R. (1974). Learning a second language through commands: The second field test. The Modern Language Journal, 58(1–2), 24–32. http://doi.org/10.2307/323986
Bagdasaryan, J., & Le Van Quyen, M. (2013). Experiencing your brain:
        Neurofeedback as a new bridge between neuroscience and phenomenology. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, Article 680.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Prentice Hall.
Berger, H. (1929). Ṻber das electrenenkephalogramm des menschen [On the     electroencephalogram of humans]. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 87(1), 527–570. http://doi.org/10.1007/BF01797193
Butnik, S. M. (2005). Neurofeedback in adolescents and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(5), 621–625. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20124
Carter, T., Pascoe, M., Bastounis, A., Morres, I. D., Callaghan, P., & Parker, A. G. (2021). The effect of physical activity on anxiety in children and young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 285, 10–21. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.02.026
Caton, R. (1875). The electric currents of the brain. The British Medical Journal, 2, 278. https://echo.mpiwg-berlin.mpg.de/ECHOdocuView?url=/permanent/vlp/lit27690/index.meta&ww=0.5359&wh=0.294&wx=0.0119
Charach, A., Skyba, A., Cook, L., & Antle, B. J. (2006). Using stimulant medication for children with ADHD: What do parents say? A brief report. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 15(2), 75–83.  https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2277288/pdf/ccap15_2p 75.pdf  
Chettaoui, N., Atia, A., & Bouhlel, M. S. (2022). Examining the effects of embodied interaction modalities on students’ retention skills in a real classroom context. Journal of Computers in Education, 9(4), 549–569. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40692-021-00213-9
Darabi, Z., Ahmadi Vasmehjani, A., Darand, M., Sangouni, A. A., Hosseinzadeh, M. (2022). Adherence to Mediterranean diet and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children: A case control study. Clinical Nutrition ESPEN, 47, 346–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clnesp.2021.11.014
De La Cruz, G., Ullauri-Moreno, M. I., & Freire, J. (2020). Estrategias didácticas para la enseñanza de inglés como lengua extranjera (EFL) dirigidas a estudiantes con trastorno por défcit de atención e hiperactividad (TDAH) [Strategies of teaching English as a Foreign Language towards Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Students (ADHD)]. Colombian Applied Linguistics Journal, 22(2), 169–182.
https://doi.org/10.14483/22487085.16118
De Meyer, H., Beckers, T., Tripp, G., & van der Oord, S. (2019). Reinforcement contingency learning in children with ADHD: Back to the basics of behavior therapy. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(12), 1889–1902. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-019-00572-z
Ellingsen, H., Olaussen, J. O., & Utne, I. B. (2009). Environmental analysis of the Norwegian fishery and aquaculture industry—A preliminary study focusing     on farmed salmon. Marine Policy, 33(3), 479–488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.marpol.2008.11.003
Enriquez-Geppert, S., Huster, R. J., Ros, T., & Wood, G. (2017). Neurofeedback. In L. S. Colzato (Ed.), Theory-driven approaches to cognitive enhancement (pp. 147–166). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-57505-6
Enriquez-Geppert, S., Smit, D., Pimenta, M. G., & Arns, M. (2019). Neurofeedback as a treatment intervention in ADHD: Current evidence and practice. Current Psychiatry Reports, 21(6), Article 46. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-019-1021-4
Ercan, E. S., Bilaç, Ö., Uysal Özaslan, T., & Rohde, L. A. (2015). Is the prevalence of ADHD in Turkish elementary school children really high? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 50(7), 1145–1152. http://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-015-1071-9
Finley, W. W., & Johnson, G. (1983). Operant control of auditory brainstem potentials in man. International Journal of Neuroscience, 21(3–4), 161–170. http://doi.org/10.3109/00207458308986134
Franza, F., Solomita, B., Pellegrino, F., & Aldi, G. (2018). Relationship between cognitive remediation and evaluation tools in clinical routine. Psychiatria Danubina, 30(7), 405–408. https://www.psychiatria-danubina.com/UserDocsImages/pdf/dnb_vol30_noSuppl%207/dnb_vol30_noSuppl%207_405.pdf
Ghadamgahi Sani, N., Akbarfahimi, M., Akbari, S., Alizadeh Zarei, M., & Taghizadeh, G. (2022). Neurofeedback training versus perceptual-motor exercises interventions in visual attention for children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, 13(2), 215–224.  http://doi.org/10.32598/bcn.2021.563.2
Ghaziri, J., Tucholka, A., Larue, V., Blanchette-Sylvestre, M., Reyburn, G., Gilbert, G., Lévesque, J., & Beauregard, M. (2013). Neurofeedback training induces changes in white and gray matter. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 44(4), 265–272. http://doi.org/10.1177/1550059413476031
Hajehforoush, E., Foroozandeh, E., Mirhosseini, H., & Abedi, A. (2018). Comparison of the effect of neurofeedback with neurofeedback plus Barkly’s parental training on auditory attention dimensions and comprehension   among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Avicenna Journal of Clinical Medicine, 25(3), 142–150. http://doi.org/10.21859/ajcm.25.3.142
Hakim Shooshtari, M., Shariati, B., Kamalzadeh, L., Naserbakht, M., Tayefi, B., & Taban, M. (2021). The prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Iran: An updated systematic review. Medical Journal of the Islamic Republic of Iran, 35(1), 60–71. https://doi.org/10.47176/mjiri.35.8
Hamilton, S. J. (2017). The effects of pointing gestures on visual attention [Master’s    thesis, Georgia Southern University]. Digital Commons@Georgia Southern. https://digitalcommons.georgiasouthern.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1300&context=honors-theses
Hammond, D. C. (2011). What is neurofeedback: An update. Journal of Neurotherapy: Investigations in Neuromodulation, Neurofeedback and Applied Neuroscience, 15(4), 305–336.
http://doi.org/10.1080/10874208.2011.623090
Harpin, V., Mazzone, L., Raynaud, J. P., Kahle, J., & Hodgkins, P. (2016). Long-term outcomes of ADHD: A systematic review of self-esteem and social function. Journal of Attention Disorders, 20(4), 295–305.
http://doi.org/10.1177/1087054713486516 
Hochstein, S., & Pavlovskaya, M. (2020). Perceptual learning of ensemble and outlier perception. Journal of Vision, 20(8), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.8.13
Huber, J. T., & Gillaspy, M. L. (Eds.). (2011). Encyclopedic dictionary of AIDS-related terminology. Routledge. 
IVA+Plus (n.d.). IVA+Plus core ADHD interpretive report. Retrieved from
https://docplayer.net/20616484-Iva-plus-standard-interpretive-report.html
Jiang, Y., Gurm, M., & Johnston, C. (2014). Child impairment and parenting self-efficacy in relation to mothers’ views of ADHD treatments. Journal of
Attention Disorders
, 18(6), 532–541.
http://doi.org/10.1177/1087054712443412
Kamiya, J. (1962, April). Conditioned discrimination of the EEG alpha rhythm in humans [Paper presentation]. Western Psychological Association, San Francisco, CA, United States.
Keilow, M., Wu, C., & Obel, C. (2020). Cumulative social disadvantage and risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Results from a nationwide cohort study.       SSM - Population Health, 10, Article 100548. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2020.100548
Koch, F.-S., Sundqvist, A., Herbert, J., Tjus, T., & Heimann, M. (2018). Changes in infant visual attention when observing repeated actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 50, 189–197. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.01.003
Kováčiková, E., & Reid, E. (2018). Brain gym exercises as mindful practices in English lessons. In H. Böttger, K. Jensen, & T. Jensen (Eds.), Mindful evolution. Conference proceedings Eichstatt 2016 and Thessaloniki 2017 (pp. 114–122). Verlag Julius Klinkhardt.
Kropotov, J. D. (2016). Functional neuromarkers for psychiatry: Applications for diagnosis and treatment. Elsevier Inc.
Louthrenoo, O., Boonchooduang, N., Likhitweerawong, N., Charoenkwan, K., & Srisurapanont, M. (2022). The effects of neurofeedback on executive functioning in children with ADHD: A meta-analysis. Journal of Attention Disorders, 26(7), 976–984. https://doi.org/10.1177/10870547211045738
Luque, D., Beesley, T., Morris, R. W., Jack, B. N., Griffiths, O., Whitford, T. J., & Le Pelley, M. E. (2017). Goal-directed and habit-like modulations of
stimulus processing during reinforcement learning. The Journal of
Neuroscience
, 37(11), 3009–3017.
http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3205-16.2017
Macedonia, M., & Mueller, K. (2016). Exploring the neural representation of novel words learned through enactment in a word recognition task. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 953.
http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00953   
Maftoon, P., & Najafi Sarem, S. (2012). The realization of Gardner’s multiple intelligences (MI) theory in second language acquisition (SLA). Journal of Language Teaching and Research, 3(6), 1233–1241.
Maftoon, P., & Taie, M. (2016). Language curriculum planning for the third millennium: A future perspective. International Journal of English Linguistics, 6(4), 41–51. http://doi.org/10.5539/ijel.v6n4p41
Márquez, N. (2011). Learning with movements: Beginning lessons in English, based on James J. Asher’s total physical response, known world-wide as TPR. Sky Oaks Publications, Inc.
McEwan, B. (2020). Sampling and validity. Annals of the International Communication Association, 44(3), 235–247. http://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2020.1792793
Mirifar, A., Keil, A., & Ehrlenspiel, F. (2022). Neurofeedback and neural self-regulation: A new perspective based on allostasis. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 33(6), 607–629. https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro-2021-0133
Nagels, A., Kelly, S. D., Kircher, T., & Straube, B. (2018).  Hand gestures alert auditory cortices: Possible impacts of learning on foreign language processing. In O. Zlatkin-Troitschanskaia, G. Wittum, & A. Dengel (Eds.), Positive learning in the age of information: A blessing or a curse? (pp. 53–66). Springer VS. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-19567-0
Nunan, D. (2011). Teaching English to young learners (1st ed.). Anaheim University Press.  
Ogrim, G., & Hestad, K. A. (2013). Effects of neurofeedback versus stimulant medication in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A randomized pilot study. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 23(7), 448–457. http://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2012.0090
Pramesti, K. N. F. (2021). The implementation of total physical response (TPR) in English learning for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder students. Journal          of Educational Study, 1(2), 87–98. https://doi.org/10.36663/joes.v1i2.184
Price, R. B., Greven, I. M., Siegle, G. J., Koster, E. H. W., & De Raedt, R. (2016). A novel attention training paradigm based on operant conditioning of eye gaze: Preliminary findings. Emotion, 16(1), 110–116.
Quenneville, A. F., Kalogeropoulou, E., Nicastro, R., Weibel, S., Chanut, F., & Perroud, N. (2022). Anxiety disorders in adult ADHD: A frequent comorbidity and a risk factor for externalizing problems. Psychiatry Research, 310, Article 114423. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2022.114423 
Rahmani, E., Mahvelati, A., Alizadeh, A., Mokhayeri, Y., Rahmani, M., Zarabi, H., Hassanvandi, S. (2022). Is neurofeedback effective in children with ADHD? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurocase, 28(1), 84–95.
Rico-Picó, J., Hoyo, Á., Guerra, S., Conejero, Á., & Rueda, M. R. (2021). Behavioral and brain dynamics of executive control in relation to children’s fluid           intelligence. Intelligence, 84, Article101513. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2020.101513
Riesco-Matías, P., Yela-Bernabé, J. R., Crego, A., & Sánchez-Zaballos, E. (2021). What do meta-analyses have to say about the efficacy of neurofeedback applied to children with ADHD? Review of previous meta-analyses and a new meta-analysis. Journal of Attention Disorders, 25(4), 473–485. http://doi.org/10.1177/1087054718821731
Rosenfeld, J. P., Rudell, A. P., & Fox, S. S. (1969). Operant control of neural events in humans. Science, 165(3895), 821–823. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.165.3895.821
Rueda, M. R., Moyano, S., & Rico-Pico, J. (2021). Attention: The grounds of self-regulated cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, Article e1582. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1582
Ryan, N., & McDougall, T. (2009). Nursing children and young people with ADHD. Routledge.
Sandford, J. A., & Turner, A. (1994). Intermediate visual and auditory continuous performance test (Version 2.6). Braintrain.
Scahill, L., & Schwab-Stone, M. (2000). Epidemiology of ADHD in school-age children. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 9(3), 541–555. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10944656 
Seger, C. A. (2018). Corticostriatal foundations of habits. Current Opinion in
Behavioral Sciences
, 20, 153–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.006
Sho’ouri, N. (2021). Predicting the success rate of healthy participants in beta neurofeedback: Determining the factors affecting the success rate of individuals. Biomedical Signal Processing and Control, 69, Article 102753. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bspc.2021.102753
Silverstein, S. M., Menditto, A. A., & Stuve, P. (2001). Shaping attention span: An operant conditioning procedure to improve neurocognition and functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27(2), 247–257. http://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006871
Sroubek, A., Kelly, M., & Li, X. (2013). Inattentiveness in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience Bulletin, 29(1), 103–110.
http://doi.org/10.1007/s12264-012-1295-6
Sterman, M., & Friar, L. (1972). Suppression of seizures in an epileptic following sensorimotor EEG feedback training. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 33(1), 89–95.
Sterman, M. B., & Wyrwicka, W. (1967).  EEG correlates of sleep: Evidence for separate forebrain substrates. Brain Research, 6(1), 143–163.
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of
neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary
(3rd ed.)
Oxford University Press.
Thompson, M., & Thompson, L. (2021). Neurofeedback with biofeedback for stress management. In P. M. Lehrer & R. L. Woolfolk, Principles and practice of stress management (4th ed.) (pp. 214–263).  The Guilford Press.
Tinius, T. P. (2003). The integrated visual and auditory continuous performance
         test as a neuropsychological measure. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(5), 439–454. http://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/18.5.439
Wakefield, E., Novack, M. A., Congdon, E. L., Franconeri, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2018). Gesture helps learners learn, but not merely by guiding their visual attention. Developmental Science, 21(6), Article e12664. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12664
Wang, L.-J., Lee, S.-Y., Tsai, C.-S., Lee, M.-J., Chou, M.-C., Kuo, H.-C., & Chou, W.-J. (2021). Validity of visual and auditory attention tests for detecting             ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 25(8), 1160–1169. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054719887433
Wender, P. H., & Tomb, D. A. (2017). ADHD: A guide to understanding symptoms, causes, diagnosis, treatment, and changes over time in children, adolescents, and adults. Oxford University Press.
Wheeler, G. (2013). Language teaching through the ages. Routledge.
Wu, X., Jiang, Y., Jiang, Y., Chen, G., Chen, Y., & Bai, X. (2021). The influence of action video games on attentional functions across visual and auditory modalities. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article, 611778. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.611778
Yucha, C., & Montgomery, D. (2008). Evidence-based practice in biofeedback and neurofeedback. Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback.
Yussen, S. R. (1974). Determinants of visual attention and recall in observational learning by preschoolers and second graders. Developmental Psychology, 10(1), 93–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0035552  
Zhou, Y.-L. (2021). The effect of dialogic reading paired with multisensory learning of Chinese characters and morphological awareness skills for L2 Chinese young learners at Hong Kong kindergartens. Foreign Language Annals, 54(4), 1082–1106. https://doi.org/10.1111/flan.12570